What is Credibility?

What is Credibility in Neuroscience?

Credibility in Neuroscience

At the BNA, we believe credible work to be work that is as robust, reliable, replicable, and reproducible as possible. 

These ideas and research practices are shared by many other organisations but may be described slightly differently. You may see other organisations refer to research that is reproducible, or research that can be trusted. Although there are many different terms used, these organisations all share the same goal as us - to promote credibility in research.

To improve credibility in neuroscience, we have outlined our main commitments to promote credible research in our manifesto. To see how we are already making a difference, take a look at our activities and how you can get involved.

Credible

Adjective - from the Latin 'credere' meaning 'to believe'
'That can be believed or trusted.'

Credibility: an issue or an initiative?

The need to create more credible research has long been recognised but is now beginning to gain attention, with various different studies identifying issues in reproducibility and reliability (1-5). Interestingly, these issues are not just in neuroscience, but have been identified across multiple disciplines including economics, psychology and in education - just to name a few (6)! Some of this is to be expected, as there are natural variations across research systems, such as in the life sciences. However, common issues across all disciplines included methods in reporting and evaluating research. There are now several initiatives to tackle this and to try and create more transparent methods of research reporting and evaluation. Find out more about the initiatives we have signed up for to improve credible research.


Research practice issues

A question that is often asked is whether credibility is an issue. Over time, research culture has led to the generation of data and results from experiments whereby credibility is often an afterthought. Ask any researcher and they will be well aware of the dreaded 'publish or perish' nature of research; where the value of a researcher and their work is judged on their ability to publish high quality data in high impact journals, or else they face their research being overlooked and undervalued. While researchers generally strive for their data to be published in these high impact journals, these journals often only accept novel data, statistically significant results, or results which support the hypotheses, thus ruling out a vast number of studies. Unfortunately, this has also led to more unfavourable methods of ensuring research does get published. For example, p-hacking is now increasingly common, where researchers may trial different statistical analyses, increase sample sizes retrospectively, or misuse data analysis in other ways to ensure their results reach significance.


Another example of unfavourable research practices includes HARKing, where the researcher Hypothesises After Results are Known, attempting to negate publication bias against null results. This type of malpractice can lead to the publication of data which is unreliable, unrepeatable, and potentially even untrue. By promoting credible research, we hope to negate these practices and advocate for research integrity, where the research undertaken is a true understanding of science.


The credibility initiative

However, while there is still a long way to go to promote credible research across neuroscience and all science disciplines, it is encouraging to think of all the progress that has been made. To tackle issues such as HARKing, initiatives such as study pre-registration and registered reports are new ideas to help publish the basis to a study, including the aims, hypothesis, and methods, prior to any results being known and thus focus on the outcome of the science rather than whether the results fit a certain narrative. Practices such as open data and other open science practices have also helped to ensure that the data has not been manipulated and does provide the published results. The creation and inclusion of these credible research practices has led to a shift in research culture promoting overall research credibility. In fact, many journals are now requesting that the data from their publications be deposited in open data archives (7). Similarly, major funding bodies are also supporting these credible science practices by requesting any work funded by them are published in open access journals to ensure complete transparency (8). Together, these initiatives across different platforms within neuroscience research are tackling any issues with credibility and promoting good, credible science.

What have the neuroscience community said?

In 2019 we asked the neuroscience community about issues surrounding neuroscience and credibility (9). Here's what they said.

Neuroscience satisfaction


Overall, neuroscientists were collectively most unsatisfied with different aspects of neuroscience relating to credibility (i.e. reproducibility, data sharing/open science and time to publish), rather than with areas relating to development (including teaching) and progression. 


The area in which neuroscientists were most satisfied was career development, with other areas (such as data sharing, reward systems, and publication times) each showing a satisfaction rate of less than 25%. 



Improving neuroscience


We provided the neuroscience community with practices which supposedly increase credible research, and asked them to rate these practices as either having a positive impact, a negative impact, or no impact on the credibility of neuroscience research.


All of the suggested practices had the majority of participants agree that they would help improve credibility in neuroscience, but many of these options were also deemed to have a negative impact on credibility.


Many of the practices deemed to have a positive impact involved implementing open science practices to improve credibility in neuroscience research, with only a minority of participants thinking this would have a negative impact. These included practices such as open data, open access publishing, and open digital materials. 


Suggestions which were deemed to have a negative impact on credible neuroscience research often related to practices which involved an increased workload, such as registered reports, study pre-registration, meta-analysis. 



Barriers to credible research



Most of the neuroscience community admitted there were barriers to doing credible, open and reproducible research. The top five suggested barriers involved a lack of resources, including support, funds, incentives and training, or the fear of being scooped. While not in the top five, many participants also identified areas such as an increased workload through administrative burdens, lack of time, and a lack of direction and information as other barriers to doing credible research in neuroscience.


Summary


While the wider neuroscience community have some issues with regards to lack of training or difficulty in career progression, most issues within neuroscience are related to aspects of research credibility. Participants have identified factors such as reproducibility of research, data sharing and open science, and time taken for publication as some of the top aspects of neuroscience they are unhappy with. Participants who responded were happiest with career development (35%), and unhappiest with reproducibility (48%) and guidance on statistics (49%).



References

1. Nature, 2016: 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility.
2. The American Society for Cell Biology, 2015: ASCB Member Survey on Reproducibility.
3. The Academy of Medical Sciences, 2015: Reproducibility and reliability of biomedical research: improving research practice.
4. European Commission, 2020: Reproducibility of scientific results in the EU. 
5. Nature, 2017: A manifesto for reproducible science.
6. The Royal Society, 2020: Are replication rates the same across academic fields? Community forecasts from the DARPA SCORE programme.
7. PLOS One, 2019: Data Availability.
8. UKRI, 2021: UKRI open access policy.
9. British Neuroscience Association, 2019: Surveying the neuroscience community on open and reproducible practices. https://osf.io/y2t97/


Share by: